Remote-Url: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-021-94374-3 Retrieved-at: 2021-09-05 18:00:48.588591+00:00 1.Astington, J. W. The paradox of intention: assessing children’s meta representational understanding. InIntentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social CognitionVol. 4 (eds Malle, B. F.et al.) 85–103 (The MIT Press, 2001).2.Bratman, M.Intention, Plans, and Practical ReasonVol. 10 (Harvard University Press, 1987).Google Scholar3.Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?.Behav. Brain Sci.1, 515–526 (1978).ArticleGoogle Scholar4.Hare, B., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?.Anim. Behav.61, 139–151 (2001).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar5.Kaminski, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe.Cognition109, 224–234 (2008).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar6.Ostoji, L.et al.Can male Eurasian jays disengage from their own current desire to feed the female what she wants?.Biol. Lett.10, 20140042–20140042 (2014).ArticleGoogle Scholar7.Krupenye, C. & Call, J. Theory of mind in animals: Current and future directions.Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci.10, e1503 (2019).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar8.Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later.Trends Cogn. Sci.12, 187–192 (2008).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar9.Emery, N. J. & Clayton, N. S. Comparative social cognition.Annu. Rev. Psychol.60, 87–113 (2009).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar10.Woodward, A. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach.Cognition69, 1–34 (1998).CASPubMedArticleGoogle Scholar11.Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N. & Tomasello, M. Fourteen-through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions.Infant Behav. Dev.21, 315–330 (1998).ArticleGoogle Scholar12.Behne, T., Carpenter, M., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action.Dev. Psychol.41, 328–337 (2005).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar13.Meltzoff, A. N. Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children.Dev. Psychol.31, 838–850 (1995).PubMedPubMed CentralArticleGoogle Scholar14.Kaminski, J. & Marshall-Pescini, S.The Social Dog: Behavior and Cognition(Elsevier, 2014).Google Scholar15.Previde, E. P. & Valsecchi, P. The immaterial cord: The dog–human attachment bond. InThe Social Dog(eds. Kaminski, J. & Marshall-Pescini, S.) 165–189 (Elsevier, 2014).16.Bergström, A.et al.Origins and genetic legacy of prehistoric dogs.Science370, 557–564 (2020).PubMedPubMed CentralArticleCASGoogle Scholar17.Kaminski, J., Schulz, L. & Tomasello, M. How dogs know when communication is intended for them: How dogs know ….Dev. Sci.15, 222–232 (2012).PubMedArticlePubMed CentralGoogle Scholar18.Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action.Dev. Sci.7, 488–498 (2004).PubMedArticlePubMed CentralGoogle Scholar19.Canteloup, C. & Meunier, H. ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Tonkean macaques’ understanding of human goal-directed actions.PeerJ5, e3227 (2017).PubMedPubMed CentralArticleGoogle Scholar20.Marsh, H. L., Stavropoulos, J., Nienhuis, T. & Legerstee, M. Six- and 9-Month-Old infants discriminate between goals despite similar action patterns.Infancy15, 94–106 (2010).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar21.Péron, F., Rat-Fischer, L., Nagle, L. & Bovet, D. ‘Unwilling’versus ‘unable’: Do grey parrots understand human intentional actions?.Interact. Stud.11, 428–441 (2010).ArticleGoogle Scholar22.Phillips, W., Barnes, J. L., Mahajan, N., Yamaguchi, M. & Santos, L. R. ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Capuchin monkeys’ (Cebus apella) understanding of human intentional action: Unwilling vs. unable in capuchin monkeys.Dev. Sci.12, 938–945 (2009).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar23.Trösch, M., Bertin, E., Calandreau, L., Nowak, R. & Lansade, L. Unwilling or willing but unable: Can horses interpret human actions as goal directed?.Anim. Cogn.23, 1035–1040 (2020).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar24.Call, J., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J. & Tomasello, M. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans.J. Comp. Psychol.117, 257–263 (2003).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar25.Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Varga, O., Topál, J. & Csányi, V. Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention.Anim. Cogn.7, 144–153 (2004).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar26.Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á. & Csányi, V. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus.Behav. Process.66, 161–172 (2004).ArticleGoogle Scholar27.Bräuer, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Visual perspective taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers.Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.88, 299–317 (2004).ArticleGoogle Scholar28.Kaminski, J., Tomasello, M., Call, J. & Bräuer, J. Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human’s perspective.Behaviour146, 979–998 (2009).ArticleGoogle Scholar29.Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á. & Csányi, V. A nonverbal test of knowledge attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children.Anim. Cogn.9, 13–26 (2006).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar30.Kaminski, J., Pitsch, A. & Tomasello, M. Dogs steal in the dark.Anim. Cogn.16, 385–394 (2013).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar31.Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., Riedel, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Making inferences about the location of hidden food: Social dog, causal ape.J. Comp. Psychol.120, 38–47 (2006).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar32.Hare, B. The domestication of social cognition in dogs.Science298, 1634–1636 (2002).ADSCASPubMedArticleGoogle Scholar33.Kirchhofer, K. C., Zimmermann, F., Kaminski, J. & Tomasello, M. Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing.PLoS ONE7, e30913 (2012).ADSCASPubMedPubMed CentralArticleGoogle Scholar34.Miklósi, Á. & Soproni, K. A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture.Anim. Cogn.9, 81–93 (2006).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar35.Krause, M. A. & Mitchell, R. W. Object-choice test. InEncyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior(eds. Vonk, J. & Shackelford, T.) 1–12 (Springer International Publishing, 2018).36.Bräuer, J. What dogs understand about humans. InThe Social Dog(eds. Kaminski, J. & Marshall-Pescini, S.) 295–317 (Elsevier, 2014).37.Soproni, K., Miklósi, A., Topál, J. & Csányi, V. Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris).J. Comp. Psychol.115, 122–126 (2001).CASPubMedArticleGoogle Scholar38.Téglás, E., Gergely, A., Kupán, K., Miklósi, Á. & Topál, J. Dogs’ gaze following is tuned to human communicative signals.Curr. Biol.22, 209–212 (2012).PubMedArticleCASGoogle Scholar39.Scheider, L., Grassmann, S., Kaminski, J. & Tomasello, M. Domestic dogs use contextual information and tone of voice when following a human pointing gesture.PLoS ONE6, 76 (2011).ArticleCASGoogle Scholar40.Kaminski, J. & Nitzschner, M. Do dogs get the point? A review of dog–human communication ability.Learn. Motiv.44, 294–302 (2013).ArticleGoogle Scholar41.Marshall-Pescini, S., Ceretta, M. & Prato-Previde, E. Do domestic dogs understand human actions as goal-directed?.PLoS ONE9, 6530 (2014).ArticleCASGoogle Scholar42.Piotti, P. & Kaminski, J. Do dogs provide information helpfully?.PLoS ONE11, e0159797 (2016).PubMedPubMed CentralArticleCASGoogle Scholar43.Range, F., Viranyi, Z. & Huber, L. Selective imitation in domestic dogs.Curr. Biol.17, 868–872 (2007).CASPubMedArticlePubMed CentralGoogle Scholar44.Fugazza, C., Petro, E., Miklósi, Á. & Pogány, Á. Social learning of goal-directed actions in dogs (Canis familiaris): Imitation or emulation?.J. Comp. Psychol.133, 244 (2019).PubMedArticlePubMed CentralGoogle Scholar45.Heyes, C. Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind.Perspect. Psychol. Sci.9, 131–143 (2014).PubMedArticlePubMed CentralGoogle Scholar46.Heyes, C. Animal mindreading: What’s the problem?.Psychon. Bull. Rev.22, 313–327 (2015).PubMedArticlePubMed CentralGoogle Scholar47.Kaminski, J.et al.Do dogs distinguish rational from irrational acts?.Anim. Behav.81, 195–203 (2011).ArticleGoogle Scholar48.Kaminski, J., Neumann, M., Bräuer, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. Dogs,Canis familiaris, communicate with humans to request but not to inform.Anim. Behav.82, 651–658 (2011).ArticleGoogle Scholar49.Udell, M. A. R., Dorey, N. R. & Wynne, C. D. L. What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions.Biol. Rev.85, 327–345 (2010).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar50.Bräuer, J. I do not understand but I care: The prosocial dog.Interact. Stud.16, 341–360 (2015).ArticleGoogle Scholar51.Searle, J. R.Intentionality: An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind.(Cambridge University Press, 1983).52.Povinelli, D. J. & Vonk, J. We don’t need a microscope to explore the chimpanzee’s mind.Mind Lang.19, 1–28 (2004).ArticleGoogle Scholar53.Mariti, C.et al.Analysis of the intraspecific visual communication in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris): A pilot study on the case of calming signals.J. Vet. Behav.18, 49–55 (2017).ArticleGoogle Scholar54.Sheppard, G. & Mills, D. S. The Development of a Psychometric Scale for the Evaluation of the Emotional Predispositions of Pet Dogs.Int. J. Comp. Psychol.15, 201–222 (2002).55.Bräuer, J., Schönefeld, K. & Call, J. When do dogs help humans?.Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.148, 138–149 (2013).ArticleGoogle Scholar56.Beerda, B., Schilder, M. B. H., van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. & de Vries, H. W. Manifestations of chronic and acute stress in dogs.Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci.52, 307–319 (1997).ArticleGoogle Scholar57.Abrantes, R.Dog Language.(Dogwise Publishing, 1997).58.Bradshaw, J. W. & Nott, H. M. Social and communication behaviour of companion dogs. InThe domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour and interactions with people(ed. Serpell, J.) 115–130 (Cambridge University Press, 1995).59.Petter, M., Musolino, E., Roberts, W. A. & Cole, M. Can dogs (Canis familiaris) detect human deception?.Behav. Processes82, 109–118 (2009).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar60.Correia-Caeiro, C., Guo, K. & Mills, D. S. Perception of dynamic facial expressions of emotion between dogs and humans.Anim. Cogn.23, 465–476 (2020).PubMedPubMed CentralArticleGoogle Scholar61.Albuquerque, N.et al.Dogs recognize dog and human emotions.Biol. Lett.12, 20150883 (2016).PubMedPubMed CentralArticleCASGoogle Scholar62.Barber, A. L. A., Randi, D., Müller, C. A. & Huber, L. The processing of human emotional faces by pet and lab dogs: evidence for lateralization and experience effects.PLoS ONE11, e0152393 (2016).PubMedPubMed CentralArticleCASGoogle Scholar63.Reid, P. J. Adapting to the human world: Dogs’ responsiveness to our social cues.Behav. Processes80, 325–333 (2009).PubMedArticleGoogle Scholar64.Friard, O. & Gamba, M. BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations.Methods Ecol. Evol.7, 1325–1330 (2016).ArticleGoogle Scholar